网站导航   4000-006-150  
小站教育
新托福口语高频话题范文
学生选择在小站备考:30天 524256名,今日申请1839人    备考咨询 >>

新托福阅读背景知识学习之青蛙的两种鸣叫方式

2017年07月19日15:17 来源:小站整理
参与(1) 阅读(5819)
摘要:托福阅读考试有3篇阅读文章,每篇阅读文章的篇幅是750字左右。作为托福考试的时间紧张来说,考生阅读全文章可能会有些压力。事实上很多文章当中都会有些学术类的文章,这些大家在平时和备考过程中是可以积累一些的。这里小编就为大家整理了托福阅读青蛙的两种鸣叫方式的背景知识,希望对大

对于托福阅读的背景知识大家平时是如何进行积累的呢?小编认为在托福阅读备考过程中,对一些学术类的背景知识大家在备考时要多做储备。这里小编为大家整理了托福阅读青蛙的两种鸣叫方式方面的背景知识,大家一起来学习吧。

新托福阅读背景知识学习之青蛙的两种鸣叫方式图1

青蛙的两种鸣叫方式

2017年4月23日托福阅读内容回顾:青蛙的两种鸣叫方式,一种音量小而简单,一种音量大而复杂。雌性青蛙更喜欢后者,两个科学家对此进行对照实验,并发现能够推广。同时也说到鸣叫方式和捕食者之间的关系。

生物类。一种青蛙的两种鸣叫方式。一种音量小而简单,一种音量大而复杂。雌性青蛙更喜欢复杂的那种,提到了两个科学家为此做的对照试验,发现这一情况可以推广化。同时也讲了鸣叫方式和捕食者的关系。

参考练习:

官方真题Official 11:Begging by Nestlings

知识拓展:

Frog hearing and communication

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Frogs and toads produce a rich variety of sounds, calls, and songs during their courtship and mating rituals. The callers, usually males, make stereotyped sounds in order to advertise their location, their mating readiness and their willingness to defend their territory; listeners respond to the calls by return calling, by approach, and by going silent. These responses have been shown to be important for species recognition, mate assessment, and localization. Beginning with the pioneering experiments of Robert Capranica in the 1930s using playback techniques with normal and synthetic calls, behavioral biologists and neurobiologists have teamed up to use frogs and toads as a model system for understanding the auditory function and evolution. It is now considered an important example of the neural basis of animal behavior, because of the simplicity of the sounds, the relative ease with which neurophysiological recordings can be made from the auditory nerve, and the reliability of localization behavior. Acoustic communication is essential for the frog's survival in both territorial defense and in localization and attraction of mates. Sounds from frogs travel through the air, through water, and through the substrate. The neural basis of communication and audition gives insights into the science of sound applied to human communication.

Sound communication Behavioral ecologyFrogs are more often heard than seen, and other frogs (and researchers) rely on their calls to identify them. Depending on the region that the frog lives in, certain times of the year are better for breeding than others, and frogs may live away from the best breeding grounds when it is not the species’ mating season. During the breeding season, they congregate to the best breeding site and compete for call time and recognition. Species that have a narrow mating season due to ponds that dry up have the most vigina calls.]

Calling strategyMale-male competitionIn many frog species only males call. Each species has a distinct call, though even among the same species, different dialects are found in different regions. Although humans cannot detect the differences in dialects, frogs distinguish between regional dialects. For example, male bullfrogs can recognize the calls of their direct territorial neighbors. By ignoring the calls of these neighbors, they save energy, and only vocalize aggressively with to an intruder’s call. In this way, calls establish territories, but they also attract females.[2] Males may have a solitary call for times when there is no competition that uses less energy. During other times, when a frog must compete with hundreds or thousands of other frogs to be heard, together they perform a chorus call where each frog calls in turn, successively. The most important feature of the chorus is the shared pattern. Through this pattern, few individuals calls are drowned out. One frog’s call may be dominant and trigger the calls of the responding frogs in symphony. Interestingly, calling is linked to physical size and females may be attracted to more vigorous calls.[2] Frogs in the same region chorus within their species and between different species. Frogs of the same species will retune their frequency so it is distinct from other frogs of the same species. Different species of frogs living in the same region have more dramatically different call frequencies.[3] The frequency and durations of different species' calls vary similarly to the preference of that species' females. The neural circuity of females of different species varies.

Male-female interactionsLike the males, females can distinguish the minute differences between individual frogs. However, males and females are attuned to different parts of the advertisement call. For example, males of the onomatopoeically named coqui species are more attuned to the low frequency co part of the call, whereas females are more attuned to the high frequency qui.[4] In fact, the order of the parts does not matter. Similarly, for females of the Tungara species, the female basilar papilla is biased towards a lower-than-average “chuck” portion of a male call.[5] Experiments that measure the vocal responses and approaches shows these attenuations.

Mode of sound communication

新托福阅读背景知识学习之青蛙的两种鸣叫方式图2

Calls are often sent through the air, but other mediums have been discovered. Some species call while they are under water and the sound travels through the water. This is adaptive in a region with many species competing for air time. Narins has found female frog species that use solid surfaces, such as blades of grass and logs, upon which they tap rhythmically to attract mates. Also, Feng has found that some species of frogs use ultrasound.

Sound production

The smallest frogs must consume lots of energy to produce calls. In addition, vocalizing muscles can make up 15% of a male spring peeper’s body mass, while the same muscles are only 3% of females. Frogs produce sound from the air sac below their mouth that from the outside, is seen to inflate and deflate. Air from the lungs is channeled to the air sac, which resonates to make the sound louder. The larynx is larger and more developed in males, though not significantly different from females.[5]

Sound localizationBiologists[who?] believed that frogs ears are placed too close together to localize sound accurately. Frogs cannot hear short, high frequency sounds. Sound is localized by the time difference when the sound reaches each ear. The “vibration spot” near the lungs vibrates in response to sound, and may be used as an additional measure to localize from.[2]

Applications of frog neuroethologyDr. Feng’s work applies the neuroethology of frog communication to medicine. A recent project on hearing aids is based on how female frogs find their mates. Females must recognize the male they choose by his call. By localizing where his call is coming from she can find him. An additional challenge is that she is localizing his call while listening to the many other frogs in the chorus, and to the noise of the stream and insects. The breeding pond is a very noisy place, and females must distinguish a male’s calls from the other noise. How they recognize the sound pattern of the male they are pursuing from the surrounding noise is similar to how intelligent hearing aids help people hear certain sounds and cancel out others. The underlying neural mechanisms are fast neural oscillations, and synaptic inhibition to cancel out noise. The timing and frequency of the sound also play a part in frog communication and may be used in Feng’s work. He also studies bat echolocation to create intelligent hearing aids. He is also working on cochlear implants.[6]

See alsoNeuroethology

Frogs

Umwelt

Vision in toads

Animal echolocation

ReferencesJump up^ Capranica (1965)

^ Jump up to:a b c d Long (1999)

Jump up^ Narins

Jump up^ Narins and Capranica (1980)

^ Jump up to:a b McClelland, Wilczynski, and Rand

Jump up^ Feng, 2007

Notes

Capranica, Robert R. (1965) The Evoked Vocal Response of the Bullfrog. MIT PRESS, Cambridge, MA. (110p.)

Albert S. Feng. Neuroscience Program University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. 17 Dec 2007

Long, Kim. Frogs A Wildlife Handbook. Boulder, Colorado: Johnson Printing, 1999.

Mundry, KM, and RR Capranica. "Correlation between auditory evoked responses in the thalamus and species-specific call characteristics. I Rana catesbeiana." Journal of Comp Physiology 160(1987): (4):477-89.

McClelland,BE., W. Wilczynski, and AS. Rand. Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Sexual dimorphism and species differences in the neurophysiology and morphology of the acoustic communication system of two neotropical hylids.

Narins, PM, and RR Capranica. "Neural adaptations for processing the two-note call of the Puerto Rican treefrog, Eleutherodactylus coqui." Brain Behavioral Evolution 17(1)(1980): 48-66.

以上就是小编为大家整理的让他跑阅读青蛙的两种鸣叫方式背景知识,大家积累起来吧。最后,小编预祝大家托福考试取得理想的成绩。

本文部分内容来源于互联网,如有疑问请联系小站管理员进行删除。

特别申明:本文内容来源网络,版权归原作者所有,如有侵权请立即与我们联系contactus@zhan.com,我们将及时处理。
看完仍有疑问?想要更详细的答案?
备考问题一键咨询提分方案
获取专业解答

相关文章

【真题练习】官方真题Official8托福阅读词汇题汇总 官方真题Official7托福阅读词汇真题练习——prof... 官方真题Official6托福阅读词汇真题练习——reta... 官方真题Official6托福阅读词汇真题练习——accu... 官方真题Official8托福阅读词汇真题练习——cope... 官方真题Official8托福阅读词汇真题练习——pred... 官方真题Official8托福阅读词汇真题练习——reli... 官方真题Official7托福阅读词汇真题练习——sphe...
小站教育托福官方群

群号:857201332

「扫二维码 加入群聊」
加入
托福关键词
版权申明| 隐私保护| 意见反馈| 联系我们| 关于我们| 网站地图| 最新资讯
© 2011-2024 ZHAN.com All Rights Reserved. 沪ICP备13042692号-23 举报电话:4000-006-150
沪公网安备 31010602002658号
增值电信业务经营许可证:沪B2-20180682